TY - JOUR
T1 - The relation between sigma power and internalizing problems across development
AU - Bothe, Kathrin
AU - Hahn, Michael A
AU - Wilhelm, Ines
AU - Hoedlmoser, Kerstin
N1 - Funding Information:
Furthermore, studies investigating young participants (< 25 years old) directly linked frontal sigma power to white matter integrity (WMI) in frontal white matter tracts (Gaudreault et al., 2018; Piantoni et al., 2013). The development of WMI in fronto-subcortical and cortico-cortical pathways increases throughout adolescence and supports top-down executive control of behavior as well as emotion regulation, both of which are known to be compromised by internalizing problems (Arain et al., 2013; Asato et al., 2010; Stoycos et al., 2017). Additionally, a study by De Witte and Mueller (2017) showed that reduced WMI in networks known to be critically involved in affective disorders (e.g. default mode network, DMN) was related to more severe symptoms of anxiety and depression in healthy young participants.The authors are highly grateful to Ann-Kathrin Joechner for her help with data collection. This work was supported by the Austrian Science Fund (T397-B02, P25000) and the Jacobs Foundation (JS1112H).
Funding Information:
This work was supported by the Austrian Science Fund ( T397-B02 , P25000 ) and the Jacobs Foundation ( JS1112H ).
Publisher Copyright:
© 2021
Copyright:
Copyright 2021 Elsevier B.V., All rights reserved.
PY - 2021/3
Y1 - 2021/3
N2 - Internalizing problems are characterized by deficits in emotion processing and regulation. They are among the most common problems in children and adolescents and mark an increased risk for depressive and anxiety disorders in later life. First evidence suggests that sleep alterations are related to the development and/or persistence of mood and anxiety disorders in children, adolescents, and adults. Most recently, data from clinical samples showed that brain activity in the sigma frequency band (9–16 Hz, i.e. sleep spindle frequency) is associated with internalizing problems in children and adolescents. However, less is known about the association between sigma power and internalizing problems in healthy participants within this age group. Here, we re-analyzed longitudinal data (25 healthy subjects (18 females) at two time points (T1: childhood mean age: 9.52 ± 0.77; T2: adolescence mean age: 16.08 ± 0.91) by correlating sigma power with measures for internalizing problems. Moreover, we calculated sigma power ratios (frontal/central, frontal/parietal, frontal/occipital) to examine whether these measures would reflect developmental changes more accurately. We found that higher values of internalizing problems at T1 were related to a lower decrease in sigma power from T1 to T2 at frontal and central derivations. Furthermore, higher values of internalizing problems at T1 as well as at T2 were related to higher sigma power ratios at T2. We suggest that sigma power may reflect maturational processes (e.g. network efficiency, integrity) related to the development of internalizing problems. In particular, a stronger decrease in frontal sigma power from childhood to adolescence may indicate a healthier development. Thus, our results emphasize the role of sigma power as a useful marker for internalizing problems during adolescence.
AB - Internalizing problems are characterized by deficits in emotion processing and regulation. They are among the most common problems in children and adolescents and mark an increased risk for depressive and anxiety disorders in later life. First evidence suggests that sleep alterations are related to the development and/or persistence of mood and anxiety disorders in children, adolescents, and adults. Most recently, data from clinical samples showed that brain activity in the sigma frequency band (9–16 Hz, i.e. sleep spindle frequency) is associated with internalizing problems in children and adolescents. However, less is known about the association between sigma power and internalizing problems in healthy participants within this age group. Here, we re-analyzed longitudinal data (25 healthy subjects (18 females) at two time points (T1: childhood mean age: 9.52 ± 0.77; T2: adolescence mean age: 16.08 ± 0.91) by correlating sigma power with measures for internalizing problems. Moreover, we calculated sigma power ratios (frontal/central, frontal/parietal, frontal/occipital) to examine whether these measures would reflect developmental changes more accurately. We found that higher values of internalizing problems at T1 were related to a lower decrease in sigma power from T1 to T2 at frontal and central derivations. Furthermore, higher values of internalizing problems at T1 as well as at T2 were related to higher sigma power ratios at T2. We suggest that sigma power may reflect maturational processes (e.g. network efficiency, integrity) related to the development of internalizing problems. In particular, a stronger decrease in frontal sigma power from childhood to adolescence may indicate a healthier development. Thus, our results emphasize the role of sigma power as a useful marker for internalizing problems during adolescence.
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=85100059148&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.01.027
DO - 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.01.027
M3 - Journal articles
C2 - 33524677
AN - SCOPUS:85100059148
SN - 0022-3956
VL - 135
SP - 302
EP - 310
JO - Journal of Psychiatric Research
JF - Journal of Psychiatric Research
ER -